Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 25
Filtrar
Mais filtros










Base de dados
Intervalo de ano de publicação
1.
Front Neurosci ; 18: 1125597, 2024.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38894940

RESUMO

In neural prostheses, intensity modulation of a single channel (i.e., through a single stimulating electrode) has been achieved by increasing the magnitude or width of each stimulation pulse, which risks eliciting pain or paraesthesia; and by changing the stimulation rate, which leads to concurrent changes in perceived frequency. In this study, we sought to render a perception of tactile intensity and frequency independently, by means of temporal pulse train patterns of fixed magnitude, delivered non-invasively. Our psychophysical study exploits a previously discovered frequency coding mechanism, where the perceived frequency of stimulus pulses grouped into periodic bursts depends on the duration of the inter-burst interval, rather than the mean pulse rate or periodicity. When electrical stimulus pulses were organised into bursts, perceived intensity was influenced by the number of pulses within a burst, while perceived frequency was determined by the time between the end of one burst envelope and the start of the next. The perceived amplitude was modulated by 1.6× while perceived frequency was varied independently by 2× within the tested range (20-40 Hz). Thus, the sensation of intensity might be controlled independently from frequency through a single stimulation channel without having to vary the injected electrical current. This can form the basis for improving strategies in delivering more complex and natural sensations for prosthetic hand users.

2.
J Physiol ; 602(9): 2089-2106, 2024 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38544437

RESUMO

When manipulating objects, humans begin adjusting their grip force to friction within 100 ms of contact. During motor adaptation, subjects become aware of the slipperiness of touched surfaces. Previously, we have demonstrated that humans cannot perceive frictional differences when surfaces are brought in contact with an immobilised finger, but can do so when there is submillimeter lateral displacement or subjects actively make the contact movement. Similarly, in, we investigated how humans perceive friction in the absence of intentional exploratory sliding or rubbing movements, to mimic object manipulation interactions. We used a two-alternative forced-choice paradigm in which subjects had to reach and touch one surface followed by another, and then indicate which felt more slippery. Subjects correctly identified the more slippery surface in 87 ± 8% of cases (mean ± SD; n = 12). Biomechanical analysis of finger pad skin displacement patterns revealed the presence of tiny (<1 mm) localised slips, known to be sufficient to perceive frictional differences. We tested whether these skin movements arise as a result of natural hand reaching kinematics. The task was repeated with the introduction of a hand support, eliminating the hand reaching movement and minimising fingertip movement deviations from a straight path. As a result, our subjects' performance significantly declined (66 ± 12% correct, mean ± SD; n = 12), suggesting that unrestricted reaching movement kinematics and factors such as physiological tremor, play a crucial role in enhancing or enabling friction perception upon initial contact. KEY POINTS: More slippery objects require a stronger grip to prevent them from slipping out of hands. Grip force adjustments to friction driven by tactile sensory signals are largely automatic and do not necessitate cognitive involvement; nevertheless, some associated awareness of grip surface slipperiness under such sensory conditions is present and helps to select a safe and appropriate movement plan. When gripping an object, tactile receptors provide frictional information without intentional rubbing or sliding fingers over the surface. However, we have discovered that submillimeter range lateral displacement might be required to enhance or enable friction sensing. The present study provides evidence that such small lateral movements causing localised partial slips arise and are an inherent part of natural reaching movement kinematics.


Assuntos
Fricção , Movimento , Humanos , Masculino , Fenômenos Biomecânicos , Adulto , Feminino , Movimento/fisiologia , Adulto Jovem , Braço/fisiologia , Percepção do Tato/fisiologia , Dedos/fisiologia , Força da Mão/fisiologia , Tato/fisiologia , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia
3.
J Neurosci ; 43(22): 4033-4046, 2023 05 31.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37142429

RESUMO

Dexterous object manipulation depends critically on information about forces normal and tangential to the fingerpads, and also on torque associated with object orientation at grip surfaces. We investigated how torque information is encoded by human tactile afferents in the fingerpads and compared them to 97 afferents recorded in monkeys (n = 3; 2 females) in our previous study. Human data included slowly-adapting Type-II (SA-II) afferents, which are absent in the glabrous skin of monkeys. Torques of different magnitudes (3.5-7.5 mNm) were applied in clockwise and anticlockwise directions to a standard central site on the fingerpads of 34 human subjects (19 females). Torques were superimposed on a 2, 3, or 4 N background normal force. Unitary recordings were made from fast-adapting Type-I (FA-I, n = 39), and slowly-adapting Type-I (SA-I, n = 31) and Type-II (SA-II, n = 13) afferents supplying the fingerpads via microelectrodes inserted into the median nerve. All three afferent types encoded torque magnitude and direction, with torque sensitivity being higher with smaller normal forces. SA-I afferent responses to static torque were inferior to dynamic stimuli in humans, while in monkeys the opposite was true. In humans this might be compensated by the addition of sustained SA-II afferent input, and their capacity to increase or decrease firing rates with direction of rotation. We conclude that the discrimination capacity of individual afferents of each type was inferior in humans than monkeys which could be because of differences in fingertip tissue compliance and skin friction.SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT We investigated how individual human tactile nerve fibers encode rotational forces (torques) and compared them to their monkey counterparts. Human hands, but not monkey hands, are innervated by a tactile neuron type (SA-II afferents) specialized to encode directional skin strain yet, so far, torque encoding has only been studied in monkeys. We find that human SA-I afferents were generally less sensitive and less able to discriminate torque magnitude and direction than their monkey counterparts, especially during the static phase of torque loading. However, this shortfall in humans could be compensated by SA-II afferent input. This indicates that variation in afferent types might complement each other signaling different stimulus features possibly providing computational advantage to discriminate stimuli.


Assuntos
Dedos , Tato , Feminino , Humanos , Torque , Tato/fisiologia , Dedos/fisiologia , Pele/inervação , Mãos , Mecanorreceptores/fisiologia , Neurônios Aferentes/fisiologia
4.
J Neurophysiol ; 128(4): 1074-1084, 2022 Oct 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36102518

RESUMO

The perceived intensity of a vibrotactile stimulus is thought to depend on single-neuron firing rates (rate coding) and the number of active afferents (population coding). Unaddressed until now is whether the temporal relation of individual spikes also conveys information about tactile intensity. We used cutaneous electro-tactile stimulation to investigate how the temporal structure of a fixed number of spikes in a 1-s train influenced the perception of intensity. Four mean spike rates spanning the flutter and vibratory hum range (36 Hz, 60 Hz; 120 Hz, 180 Hz) were tested, with spikes grouped into a regular pattern, or bursts of 2-6 spikes spaced 3 ms apart. To link a putative neural code to perception, perceived intensity was assessed in 16 human participants (aged 20-45; 4 females) using the psychophysical paradigm of magnitude estimation. Compound sensory nerve action potentials were recorded to assess any stimulus variation in afferent recruitment. The temporal structuring of a fixed number of spikes into periodic bursts of multiple spikes altered perceived intensity as a function of burst spike count. The largest increase was seen at 36 Hz, where the bursts of six spikes were rated 2.1 times stronger than the regularly spaced spikes [95% confidence interval (CI): 1.9-2.3]. The true increase is likely larger as temporal structuring of spikes into bursts had some negative effect on afferent recruitment. We conclude that the perceived intensity can be modulated by changing temporal features of afferent discharge even when normalized for the number of recruited afferents.NEW & NOTEWORTHY Structuring a fixed number of spikes into temporal burst patterns evoke gradations of perceived intensity with burst spike count, emphasizing the importance of spike timing in primary afferents for shaping perception. This forms the basis for new strategies in communicating a range of intensity information to users of neural interfaces by simply varying the timing of spikes in nonspecific primary afferents using fixed-charge electric pulses, without requiring alterations in stimulation current or mean pulse frequency.


Assuntos
Neurônios , Tato , Potenciais de Ação/fisiologia , Potenciais Evocados , Feminino , Humanos , Neurônios/fisiologia , Neurônios Aferentes/fisiologia , Tato/fisiologia
5.
Front Neurosci ; 16: 1006185, 2022.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36161171

RESUMO

Both hearing and touch are sensitive to the frequency of mechanical oscillations-sound waves and tactile vibrations, respectively. The mounting evidence of parallels in temporal frequency processing between the two sensory systems led us to directly address the question of perceptual frequency equivalence between touch and hearing using stimuli of simple and more complex temporal features. In a cross-modal psychophysical paradigm, subjects compared the perceived frequency of pulsatile mechanical vibrations to that elicited by pulsatile acoustic (click) trains, and vice versa. Non-invasive pulsatile stimulation designed to excite a fixed population of afferents was used to induce desired temporal spike trains at frequencies spanning flutter up to vibratory hum (>50 Hz). The cross-modal perceived frequency for regular test pulse trains of either modality was a close match to the presented stimulus physical frequency up to 100 Hz. We then tested whether the recently discovered "burst gap" temporal code for frequency, that is shared by the two senses, renders an equivalent cross-modal frequency perception. When subjects compared trains comprising pairs of pulses (bursts) in one modality against regular trains in the other, the cross-sensory equivalent perceptual frequency best corresponded to the silent interval between the successive bursts in both auditory and tactile test stimuli. These findings suggest that identical acoustic and vibrotactile pulse trains, regardless of pattern, elicit equivalent frequencies, and imply analogous temporal frequency computation strategies in both modalities. This perceptual correspondence raises the possibility of employing a cross-modal comparison as a robust standard to overcome the prevailing methodological limitations in psychophysical investigations and strongly encourages cross-modal approaches for transmitting sensory information such as translating pitch into a similar pattern of vibration on the skin.

6.
Sci Rep ; 12(1): 11014, 2022 06 30.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35773321

RESUMO

When tactile afferents were manipulated to fire in periodic bursts of spikes, we discovered that the perceived pitch corresponded to the inter-burst interval (burst gap) in a spike train, rather than the spike rate or burst periodicity as previously thought. Given that tactile frequency mechanisms have many analogies to audition, and indications that temporal frequency channels are linked across the two modalities, we investigated whether there is burst gap temporal encoding in the auditory system. To link this putative neural code to perception, human subjects (n = 13, 6 females) assessed pitch elicited by trains of temporally-structured acoustic pulses in psychophysical experiments. Each pulse was designed to excite a fixed population of cochlear neurons, precluding place of excitation cues, and to elicit desired temporal spike trains in activated afferents. We tested periodicities up to 150 Hz using a variety of burst patterns and found striking deviations from periodicity-predicted pitch. Like the tactile system, the duration of the silent gap between successive bursts of neural activity best predicted perceived pitch, emphasising the role of peripheral temporal coding in shaping pitch. This suggests that temporal patterning of stimulus pulses in cochlear implant users might improve pitch perception.


Assuntos
Percepção da Altura Sonora , Tato , Estimulação Acústica , Implantes Cocleares , Feminino , Audição/fisiologia , Humanos , Masculino , Percepção da Altura Sonora/fisiologia
7.
IEEE Trans Haptics ; 15(1): 14-19, 2022.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34990370

RESUMO

It has been suggested that tactile intensity perception can be explained by a linear function of spike rate weighted by afferent type. Other than relying on mathematical models, verifying this experimentally is difficult due to the frequency tuning of different afferent types and changes in population recruitment patterns with vibrotactile frequency. To overcome these complexities, we used pulsatile mechanical stimuli which activate the same afferent population regardless of the repetition rate (frequency), generating one action potential per pulse. We used trains of different frequencies (20-200 Hz) to investigate perceived intensity. Subjects' magnitude ratings increased with pulse rate up to ∼100 Hz and plateaued beyond this frequency. This was true regardless of pulse amplitude, from small pulses that exclusively activated Pacinian (PC) afferents, to pulses large enough to activate other afferents including slowly adapting. Electrical stimulation, which activates afferents indiscriminately, plateaued at a similar frequency, although not in all subjects. As the plateauing did not depend on indentation magnitude and hence on afferent weights, we propose that the contribution of spike count to intensity perception is weighted by a function of frequency. This may explain why fine textures evoking high frequency vibrations of a small magnitude do not feel disproportionally intense.


Assuntos
Percepção do Tato , Tato , Potenciais de Ação , Estimulação Elétrica , Humanos , Mecanorreceptores/fisiologia , Neurônios Aferentes/fisiologia , Tato/fisiologia , Vibração
8.
IEEE Trans Haptics ; 15(1): 20-25, 2022.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34982692

RESUMO

Human tactile perception and motor control rely on the frictional estimates that stem from the deformation of the skin and slip events. However, it is not clear how exactly these mechanical events relate to the perception of friction. This study aims to quantify how minor lateral displacement and speed enables subjects to feel frictional differences. In a 2-alternative forced-choice protocol, an ultrasonic friction-reduction device was brought in contact perpendicular to the skin surface of an immobilized index finger; after reaching 1N normal force, the plate was moved laterally. A combination of four displacement magnitudes (0.2, 0.5, 1.2 and 2 mm), two levels of friction (high, low) and three displacement speeds (1, 5 and 10 mm/s) were tested. We found that the perception of frictional difference was enabled by submillimeter range lateral displacement. Friction discrimination thresholds were reached with lateral displacements ranging from 0.2 to 0.5 mm and surprisingly speed had only a marginal effect. These results demonstrate that partial slips are sufficient to cause awareness of surface slipperiness. These quantitative data are crucial for designing haptic devices that render slipperiness. The results also show the importance of subtle lateral finger movements present during dexterous manipulation tasks.


Assuntos
Percepção do Tato , Dedos , Fricção , Humanos , Movimento , Pele
9.
J Cogn Neurosci ; 33(12): 2427-2439, 2021 11 05.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34424986

RESUMO

Sensory suppression refers to the phenomenon that sensory input generated by our own actions, such as moving a finger to press a button to hear a tone, elicits smaller neural responses than sensory input generated by external agents. This observation is usually explained via the internal forward model in which an efference copy of the motor command is used to compute a corollary discharge, which acts to suppress sensory input. However, because moving a finger to press a button is accompanied by neural processes involved in preparing and performing the action, it is unclear whether sensory suppression is the result of movement planning, movement execution, or both. To investigate this, in two experiments, we compared ERPs to self-generated tones that were produced by voluntary, semivoluntary, or involuntary button-presses, with externally generated tones that were produced by a computer. In Experiment 1, the semivoluntary and involuntary button-presses were initiated by the participant or experimenter, respectively, by electrically stimulating the median nerve in the participant's forearm, and in Experiment 2, by applying manual force to the participant's finger. We found that tones produced by voluntary button-presses elicited a smaller N1 component of the ERP than externally generated tones. This is known as N1-suppression. However, tones produced by semivoluntary and involuntary button-presses did not yield significant N1-suppression. We also found that the magnitude of N1-suppression linearly decreased across the voluntary, semivoluntary, and involuntary conditions. These results suggest that movement planning is a necessary condition for producing sensory suppression. We conclude that the most parsimonious account of sensory suppression is the internal forward model.


Assuntos
Eletroencefalografia , Potenciais Evocados , Dedos , Humanos , Movimento
10.
J Neurophysiol ; 125(2): 687-692, 2021 02 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33439792

RESUMO

Perceived frequency of vibrotactile stimuli can be divided into two distinctive cutaneous sensations-flutter (<60 Hz) and vibratory hum (>60 Hz), mediated by two different tactile afferent types [fast adapting type I (FA1) and fast adapting type II (FA2), respectively]. We recently demonstrated a novel form of neural coding in the human tactile system, where frequency perception of stimulus pulses grouped into periodic bursts in the flutter range depended on the duration of the silent gap between bursts, rather than the periodicity or mean impulse rate. Here, we investigated whether this interburst interval could also explain the perceived frequency of electrocutaneous pulse patterns delivered at frequencies above the flutter range. At stimulus rates of 50 to 190 pulses/s, the burst gap model correctly predicted the perceived frequency. This shows that the burst gap code represents a general coding strategy that spans the range of frequencies traditionally attributed to two different tactile channels.NEW & NOTEWORTHY We present evidence for a generalized frequency processing strategy on tactile afferent inputs that is shared across a broad range of frequencies extending beyond the flutter range, supporting the notion that spike timing has an important role in shaping tactile perception.


Assuntos
Percepção do Tato , Tato , Adolescente , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Mecanorreceptores/classificação , Mecanorreceptores/fisiologia , Pele/citologia , Pele/inervação , Córtex Somatossensorial/citologia , Córtex Somatossensorial/fisiologia , Vibração , Adulto Jovem
11.
J Comp Neurol ; 529(1): 187-220, 2021 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32374027

RESUMO

The dorsal column nuclei complex (DCN-complex) includes the dorsal column nuclei (DCN, referring to the gracile and cuneate nuclei collectively), external cuneate, X, and Z nuclei, and the median accessory nucleus. The DCN are organized by both somatotopy and modality, and have a diverse range of afferent inputs and projection targets. The functional organization and connectivity of the DCN implicate them in a variety of sensorimotor functions, beyond their commonly accepted role in processing and transmitting somatosensory information to the thalamus, yet this is largely underappreciated in the literature. To consolidate insights into their sensorimotor functions, this review examines the morphology, organization, and connectivity of the DCN and their associated nuclei. First, we briefly discuss the receptors, afferent fibers, and pathways involved in conveying tactile and proprioceptive information to the DCN. Next, we review the modality and somatotopic arrangements of the remaining constituents of the DCN-complex. Finally, we examine and discuss the functional implications of the myriad of DCN-complex projection targets throughout the diencephalon, midbrain, and hindbrain, in addition to their modulatory inputs from the cortex. The organization and connectivity of the DCN-complex suggest that these nuclei should be considered a complex integration and distribution hub for sensorimotor information.


Assuntos
Bulbo/fisiologia , Rede Nervosa/fisiologia , Córtex Somatossensorial/fisiologia , Corno Dorsal da Medula Espinal/fisiologia , Tálamo/fisiologia , Animais , Humanos , Bulbo/anatomia & histologia , Rede Nervosa/anatomia & histologia , Córtex Somatossensorial/anatomia & histologia , Corno Dorsal da Medula Espinal/anatomia & histologia , Tálamo/anatomia & histologia , Tato/fisiologia
12.
PLoS One ; 15(8): e0237440, 2020.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32790784

RESUMO

We have previously described a novel temporal encoding mechanism in the somatosensory system, where mechanical pulses grouped into periodic bursts create a perceived tactile frequency based on the duration of the silent gap between bursts, rather than the mean rate or the periodicity. This coding strategy may offer new opportunities for transmitting information to the brain using various sensory neural prostheses and haptic interfaces. However, it was not known whether the same coding mechanisms apply when using electrical stimulation, which recruits a different spectrum of afferents. Here, we demonstrate that the predictions of the burst gap coding model for frequency perception apply to burst stimuli delivered with electrical pulses, re-emphasising the importance of the temporal structure of spike patterns in neural processing and perception of tactile stimuli. Reciprocally, the electrical stimulation data confirm that the results observed with mechanical stimulation do indeed depend on neural processing mechanisms in the central nervous system, and are not due to skin mechanical factors and resulting patterns of afferent activation.


Assuntos
Estimulação Elétrica , Percepção do Tato/fisiologia , Potenciais de Ação , Adulto , Axônios/fisiologia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Células Receptoras Sensoriais/fisiologia , Adulto Jovem
13.
Front Neurosci ; 14: 500, 2020.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32508581

RESUMO

The temporal pattern of action potentials can convey rich information in a variety of sensory systems. We describe a new non-invasive technique that enables precise, reliable generation of action potential patterns in tactile peripheral afferent neurons by brief taps on the skin. Using this technique, we demonstrate sophisticated coding of temporal information in the somatosensory system, that shows that perceived vibration frequency is not encoded in peripheral afferents as was expected by either their firing rate or the underlying periodicity of the stimulus. Instead, a burst gap or silent gap between trains of action potentials conveys frequency information. This opens the possibility of new encoding strategies that could be deployed to convey sensory information using mechanical or electrical stimulation in neural prostheses and brain-machine interfaces, and may extend to senses beyond artificial encoding of aspects of touch. We argue that a focus on appropriate use of effective temporal coding offers more prospects for rapid improvement in the function of these interfaces than attempts to scale-up existing devices.

14.
Elife ; 82019 08 06.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31383258

RESUMO

The established view is that vibrotactile stimuli evoke two qualitatively distinctive cutaneous sensations, flutter (frequencies < 60 Hz) and vibratory hum (frequencies > 60 Hz), subserved by two distinct receptor types (Meissner's and Pacinian corpuscle, respectively), which may engage different neural processing pathways or channels and fulfil quite different biological roles. In psychological and physiological literature, those two systems have been labelled as Pacinian and non-Pacinian channels. However, we present evidence that low-frequency spike trains in Pacinian afferents can readily induce a vibratory percept with the same low frequency attributes as sinusoidal stimuli of the same frequency, thus demonstrating a universal frequency decoding system. We achieved this using brief low-amplitude pulsatile mechanical stimuli to selectively activate Pacinian afferents. This indicates that spiking pattern, regardless of receptor type, determines vibrotactile frequency perception. This mechanism may underlie the constancy of vibrotactile frequency perception across different skin regions innervated by distinct afferent types.


Assuntos
Potenciais de Ação , Mecanorreceptores/fisiologia , Limiar Sensorial , Percepção do Tato , Tato , Adulto , Feminino , Voluntários Saudáveis , Humanos , Masculino , Adulto Jovem
15.
Front Syst Neurosci ; 13: 11, 2019.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30983977

RESUMO

The brainstem dorsal column nuclei (DCN) are essential to inform the brain of tactile and proprioceptive events experienced by the body. However, little is known about how ascending somatosensory information is represented in the DCN. Our objective was to investigate the usefulness of high-frequency (HF) and low-frequency (LF) DCN signal features (SFs) in predicting the nerve from which signals were evoked. We also aimed to explore the robustness of DCN SFs and map their relative information content across the brainstem surface. DCN surface potentials were recorded from urethane-anesthetized Wistar rats during sural and peroneal nerve electrical stimulation. Five salient SFs were extracted from each recording electrode of a seven-electrode array. We used a machine learning approach to quantify and rank information content contained within DCN surface-potential signals following peripheral nerve activation. Machine-learning of SF and electrode position combinations was quantified to determine a hierarchy of information importance for resolving the peripheral origin of nerve activation. A supervised back-propagation artificial neural network (ANN) could predict the nerve from which a response was evoked with up to 96.8 ± 0.8% accuracy. Guided by feature-learnability, we maintained high prediction accuracy after reducing ANN algorithm inputs from 35 (5 SFs from 7 electrodes) to 6 (4 SFs from one electrode and 2 SFs from a second electrode). When the number of input features were reduced, the best performing input combinations included HF and LF features. Feature-learnability also revealed that signals recorded from the same midline electrode can be accurately classified when evoked from bilateral nerve pairs, suggesting DCN surface activity asymmetry. Here we demonstrate a novel method for mapping the information content of signal patterns across the DCN surface and show that DCN SFs are robust across a population. Finally, we also show that the DCN is functionally asymmetrically organized, which challenges our current understanding of somatotopic symmetry across the midline at sub-cortical levels.

16.
Med Sci Educ ; 29(3): 625-629, 2019 Sep.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34457524

RESUMO

Concept and knowledge maps have been shown to improve students' learning by emphasising meaningful relationships between phenomena. A user-friendly online tool that enables assessment of students' maps with automated feedback might therefore have significant benefits for learning. For that purpose, we developed an online software platform known as Knowledge Maps. Two pilot studies were performed to evaluate the usability and efficacy of Knowledge Maps. Study A demonstrated significantly improved perceptions of learning after using Knowledge Maps to learn pathology. Study B showed significant improvement between pre-test and post-test scores in an anatomy course. These preliminary studies indicate that this software is readily accepted and may have potential benefits for learning.

17.
Curr Biol ; 27(10): 1485-1490.e2, 2017 May 22.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28479322

RESUMO

Skin vibrations sensed by tactile receptors contribute significantly to the perception of object properties during tactile exploration [1-4] and to sensorimotor control during object manipulation [5]. Sustained low-frequency skin vibration (<60 Hz) evokes a distinct tactile sensation referred to as flutter whose frequency can be clearly perceived [6]. How afferent spiking activity translates into the perception of frequency is still unknown. Measures based on mean spike rates of neurons in the primary somatosensory cortex are sufficient to explain performance in some frequency discrimination tasks [7-11]; however, there is emerging evidence that stimuli can be distinguished based also on temporal features of neural activity [12, 13]. Our study's advance is to demonstrate that temporal features are fundamental for vibrotactile frequency perception. Pulsatile mechanical stimuli were used to elicit specified temporal spike train patterns in tactile afferents, and subsequently psychophysical methods were employed to characterize human frequency perception. Remarkably, the most salient temporal feature determining vibrotactile frequency was not the underlying periodicity but, rather, the duration of the silent gap between successive bursts of neural activity. This burst gap code for frequency represents a previously unknown form of neural coding in the tactile sensory system, which parallels auditory pitch perception mechanisms based on purely temporal information where longer inter-pulse intervals receive higher perceptual weights than short intervals [14]. Our study also demonstrates that human perception of stimuli can be determined exclusively by temporal features of spike trains independent of the mean spike rate and without contribution from population response factors.


Assuntos
Neurônios/fisiologia , Percepção/fisiologia , Fenômenos Fisiológicos da Pele , Córtex Somatossensorial/fisiologia , Tato/fisiologia , Potenciais de Ação , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Periodicidade , Psicofísica , Limiar Sensorial , Vibração , Adulto Jovem
18.
BMC Neurosci ; 16: 52, 2015 Aug 13.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26268809

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: A myriad of studies have argued that tactile sensibility is underpinned exclusively by large myelinated mechanoreceptors. However, the functional significance of their slow-conducting counterparts, termed C-low threshold mechanoreceptors (C-LTMRs), remains largely unexplored. We recently showed the emergence of brush- and vibration-evoked allodynia in human hairy and glabrous skin during background muscle pain. The allodynia persisted following the preferential blockade of myelinated fibres but was abolished by the preferential blockade of cutaneous C fibres, thereby suggesting a pathway involving hairy skin C-LTMRs and their functional counterparts in glabrous skin in this phenomenon. In the present study, we tested the effects of preferential A- and C-fibre conduction blocks and pharmacological blockade of T-type calcium channel Cav3.2 (expressed selectively on small-fibre LTMRs) on monofilament detection thresholds in healthy participants by compression, low-dose intradermal anaesthesia (xylocaine 0.25 %) and selective T-channel antagonist, TTA-A2. RESULTS: We found that all participants could detect monofilament contacts (as low as 1.6 mN) within the innocuous tactile range regardless of the preferential blockade of myelinated fibres. Furthermore, during the compression block no subject reported a switch in modality from touch to pain. That is, the low-force monofilament contacts were always perceived as non-painful. However, there was a small but significant elevation of monofilament thresholds (~2 mN) in the glabrous skin following the compression block. Importantly, no differences were found in the thresholds across hairy and glabrous regions while the myelinated fibres were conducting or not. The preferential blockade of C fibres in the glabrous skin (with myelinated fibres intact) also resulted in a small but significant elevation of tactile thresholds. Furthermore, the use of T-channel blocker in the glabrous skin during compression block of myelinated fibres resulted in complete abolition of monofilament sensibility within the innocuous tactile range (tested up to ~20 mN). CONCLUSIONS: These observations suggest that C-LTMRs need not be regarded as a redundant tactile system, but appear to complement normal large-myelinated-fibre tactile function. Convergent findings in glabrous and hairy skin lend support for an underlying system of innocuous mechanoreception with Cav3.2-expressing unmyelinated fibres.


Assuntos
Benzenoacetamidas/farmacologia , Bloqueadores dos Canais de Cálcio/farmacologia , Canais de Cálcio Tipo T/metabolismo , Lidocaína/farmacologia , Mecanorreceptores/efeitos dos fármacos , Piridinas/farmacologia , Bloqueadores do Canal de Sódio Disparado por Voltagem/farmacologia , Adolescente , Adulto , Feminino , Mãos/inervação , Humanos , Masculino , Mecanorreceptores/metabolismo , Bloqueio Nervoso , Percepção da Dor/efeitos dos fármacos , Percepção da Dor/fisiologia , Estimulação Física , Limiar Sensorial/efeitos dos fármacos , Limiar Sensorial/fisiologia , Pele/efeitos dos fármacos , Pele/inervação , Percepção do Tato/efeitos dos fármacos , Percepção do Tato/fisiologia , Adulto Jovem
19.
PLoS One ; 9(9): e107617, 2014.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25215534

RESUMO

Integration of information by convergence of inputs onto sensory cortical neurons is a requisite for processing higher-order stimulus features. Convergence across defined peripheral input classes has generally been thought to occur at levels beyond the primary sensory cortex, however recent work has shown that this does not hold for the convergence of slowly-adapting and rapidly-adapting inputs in primary somatosensory cortex. We have used a new analysis method for multi-unit recordings, to show convergence of inputs deriving from the rapidly-adapting and Pacinian channels in a proportion of neurons in both primary and secondary somatosensory cortex in the anaesthetised cat. We have validated this method using single-unit recordings. The secondary somatosensory cortex has a greater proportion of sites that show convergence of this type than primary somatosensory cortex. These findings support the hypothesis that the more complex features processed in higher cortical areas require a greater degree of convergence across input classes, but also shows that this convergence is apparent in the primary somatosensory cortex.


Assuntos
Vias Aferentes/fisiologia , Neurônios Aferentes/fisiologia , Córtex Somatossensorial/fisiologia , Animais , Mapeamento Encefálico , Gatos , Potenciais Somatossensoriais Evocados/fisiologia , Tato/fisiologia
20.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24111087

RESUMO

Current prosthetic limbs are limited by a lack of tactile feedback. Slip feedback is particularly important to inform grip. Object slip is marked by both a change in the normal grip force applied and a change in force tangential to the fingertips. In this study, we demonstrate that a new multi-axial tactile sensor composed of gold nanoparticle strain gauges is able to record slip and reconstruct the X, Y, and Z forces incident on the sensor's surface due to a slipping object. We entered the X, Y, and Z force components generated by the slip event into a noisy leaky integrate and fire model to simulate the firing responses of SA1 and FA1 afferents. We also recorded a uniaxial normal force input representative of tactile contact. A single set of SA1 model and FA1 model parameters generated realistic firing patterns for both the slip and normal force recordings. These results suggest that canonical SA1 and FA1 afferent models could be used to generate biomimetic electrical stimulation patterns for both slip and touch stimuli. When used to activate the tactile afferents of an amputee, these electrical stimulation patterns could create natural and distinguishable slip and touch percepts for closed loop control of an upper limb neural prosthesis.


Assuntos
Estimulação Elétrica/métodos , Dedos/fisiologia , Neurônios/fisiologia , Próteses e Implantes , Tato/fisiologia , Desenho de Equipamento , Força da Mão/fisiologia , Humanos , Modelos Teóricos
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA
...