Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 54
Filtrar
1.
PLoS One ; 17(2): e0263879, 2022.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35148338

RESUMO

In two eye-tracking reading experiments, we used a variant of the filled gap technique to investigate how strong and weak islands are processed on a moment-to-moment basis during comprehension. Experiment 1 provided a conceptual replication of previous studies showing that real time processing is sensitive to strong islands. In the absence of an island, readers experienced processing difficulty when a pronoun appeared in a position of a predicted gap, but this difficulty was absent when the pronoun appeared inside a strong island. Experiment 2 showed an analogous effect for weak islands: a processing cost was seen for a pronoun in the position of a predicted gap in a that-complement clause, but this cost was absent in a matched whether clause, which constitutes a weak island configuration. Overall, our results are compatible with the claim that active dependency formation is suspended, or reduced, in both weak and strong island structures.


Assuntos
Compreensão/fisiologia , Tecnologia de Rastreamento Ocular/instrumentação , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Psicolinguística/instrumentação , Adulto Jovem
2.
Can J Exp Psychol ; 71(1): 71-88, 2017 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28252996

RESUMO

Reliable measurement of affective responses is critical for research into human emotion. Affective evaluation of words is most commonly gauged on multiple dimensions-including valence (positivity) and arousal-using a rating scale. Despite its popularity, this scale is open to criticism: It generates ordinal data that is often misinterpreted as interval, it does not provide the fine resolution that is essential by recent theoretical accounts of emotion, and its extremes may not be properly calibrated. In 5 experiments, the authors introduce a new slider tool for affective evaluation of words on a continuous, well-calibrated and high-resolution scale. In Experiment 1, participants were shown a word and asked to move a manikin representing themselves closer to or farther away from the word. The manikin's distance from the word strongly correlated with the word's valence. In Experiment 2, individual differences in shyness and sociability elicited reliable differences in distance from the words. Experiment 3 validated the results of Experiments 1 and 2 using a demographically more diverse population of responders. Finally, Experiment 4 (along with Experiment 2) suggested that task demand is not a potential cause for scale recalibration. In Experiment 5, men and women placed a manikin closer or farther from words that showed sex differences in valence, highlighting the sensitivity of this measure to group differences. These findings shed a new light on interactions among affect, language, and individual differences, and demonstrate the utility of a new tool for measuring word affect. (PsycINFO Database Record


Assuntos
Afeto , Psicolinguística/métodos , Psicometria/métodos , Adolescente , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Individualidade , Masculino , Psicolinguística/instrumentação , Psicometria/instrumentação , Fatores Sexuais , Adulto Jovem
3.
J Clin Exp Neuropsychol ; 36(2): 205-20, 2014.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24512631

RESUMO

Troyer and colleagues [Troyer, A. K., Moscovitch, M., & Winocur, G. (1997). Clustering and switching as two components of verbal fluency: evidence from younger and older healthy adults. Neuropsychology, 11(1), 138-146] developed a seminal method to measure clustering and switching behaviors during verbal fluency (VF) productions. We sought to expand the reach of their system by modifying the scoring rules. Compared to the Troyer system, our modifications yield comparable estimates of interrater reliability and similar patterns of correlation with demographic characteristics for both clustering and switching in healthy adults. However, two objective measures of word relatedness (interword interval timing and latent semantic analysis) confirm that our revisions capture additional information about the organization of entries in the lexical network.


Assuntos
Testes Neuropsicológicos/normas , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Adolescente , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Testes Neuropsicológicos/estatística & dados numéricos , Psicolinguística/instrumentação , Psicolinguística/métodos , Psicolinguística/normas , Reprodutibilidade dos Testes , Semântica , Adulto Jovem
4.
J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform ; 40(1): 172-85, 2014 Feb.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23750962

RESUMO

The reach-to-touch paradigm has become an increasingly popular tool in the study of human cognition. It is widely held that reaching responses are able to reveal the moment-by-moment unfolding of decision processes by virtue of an assumed continuity between reaching trajectories and the underlying "cognitive trajectory." Yet the standard analysis of reaching trajectories aggregates the trajectories across stimulus viewing times, which yields ambiguous results. Here we introduce a new version of the reach-to-touch paradigm that incorporates the response-signal procedure to elicit reaching movements across a wide range of stimulus viewing times. We then analyze the direction of the initial movement by stimulus viewing time, which produces a sigmoidal growth pattern. Of note, we show how this sigmoidal relationship between stimulus viewing time and initial direction can be used to test and constrain the dynamical claims of computational models of basic cognitive processes. We introduce our new version of the reach-to-touch paradigm and analyses in the context of a lexical decision task and we compare our results with the dynamical claims of the dual-route cascaded model of reading.


Assuntos
Modelos Teóricos , Movimento/fisiologia , Neuropsicologia/métodos , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Adulto , Humanos , Neuropsicologia/instrumentação , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos/fisiologia , Mascaramento Perceptivo/fisiologia , Psicolinguística/instrumentação , Psicolinguística/métodos , Priming de Repetição/fisiologia , Adulto Jovem
5.
J Psycholinguist Res ; 43(2): 167-85, 2014 Apr.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23516001

RESUMO

Using the eye-tracking method, the present study depicted pre- and post-head processing for simple scrambled sentences of head-final languages. Three versions of simple Japanese active sentences with ditransitive verbs were used: namely, (1) SO1O2V canonical, (2) SO2O1V single-scrambled, and (3) O1O2SV double-scrambled order. First pass reading times indicated that the third noun phrase just before the verb in both single- and double-scrambled sentences required longer reading times compared to canonical sentences. Re-reading times (the sum of all fixations minus the first pass reading) showed that all noun phrases including the crucial phrase before the verb in double-scrambled sentences required longer re-reading times than those required for single-scrambled sentences; single-scrambled sentences had no difference from canonical ones. Therefore, a single filler-gap dependency can be resolved in pre-head anticipatory processing whereas two filler-gap dependencies require much greater cognitive loading than a single case. These two dependencies can be resolved in post-head processing using verb agreement information.


Assuntos
Movimentos Oculares/fisiologia , Psicolinguística/métodos , Leitura , Adulto , Medições dos Movimentos Oculares , Feminino , Humanos , Japão , Masculino , Psicolinguística/instrumentação , Adulto Jovem
6.
J Fam Psychol ; 27(5): 691-701, 2013 Oct.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24098961

RESUMO

Recent research links first-person plural pronoun use (we-talk) by individual romantic partners to adaptive relationship functioning and individual health outcomes. To examine a possible boundary condition of adaptive we-talk in couples coping with health problems, we correlated asymmetric couple-level we/I-ratios (more we-talk relative to I-talk by the spouse than the patient) with a concurrent pattern of directional demand-withdraw (D-W) interaction in which the spouse demands change while the patient withdraws. Couples in which a partner who abused alcohol (n = 65), smoked cigarettes despite having heart or lung disease (n = 24), or had congestive heart failure (n = 58) discussed a health-related disagreement during a video-recorded interaction task. Transcripts of these conversations provided measures of pronoun use for each partner, and trained observers coded D-W patterns from the recordings. As expected, partner asymmetry in we/I-ratio scores predicted directional demand-withdraw, such that spouses who used more we-talk (relative to I-talk) than patients tended to assume the demand role in concurrent D-W interaction. Asymmetric I-talk rather than we-talk accounted for this association, and asymmetric you-talk contributed independently as well. In contrast to previous studies of we-talk by individual partners, the present results identify dyad-level pronoun patterns that clearly do not mark beneficent processes: asymmetric partner we/I-ratios and you-talk reflect problematic demand-withdraw interaction.


Assuntos
Conflito Psicológico , Nível de Saúde , Relações Interpessoais , Cônjuges/psicologia , Comportamento Verbal/fisiologia , Adaptação Psicológica/fisiologia , Adulto , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Alcoolismo/psicologia , Feminino , Insuficiência Cardíaca/psicologia , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Psicolinguística/instrumentação , Psicolinguística/métodos , Fumar/psicologia , Adulto Jovem
7.
Percept Mot Skills ; 116(2): 415-34, 2013 Apr.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24032320

RESUMO

The purpose was the develop a questionnaire to identify the specific listening difficulties of second language (L2) learners. Based on previous research, a questionnaire containing 31 items was developed and administered to 1,056 college freshmen. The whole sample was split randomly into two subsamples, each containing 528 cases. Exploratory factor analysis was performed to analyse the first subsample, and six factors were extracted, explaining a total of 57.1% of variance. To test the factor model, confirmatory factor analysis was conducted with the second subsample. Various fit indices were examined. The best fitting model for the data was a 23-item, six-factor model representing text, input channel and surroundings, relevance, listener, speaker, and task. Apart from the listener factor, all components are external ones and deemed to be uncontrollable by listeners. L2 learners must take an active role in listening practice to overcome L2 listening difficulties.


Assuntos
Idioma , Aprendizagem/fisiologia , Multilinguismo , Percepção da Fala/fisiologia , Inquéritos e Questionários/normas , Adolescente , Adulto , Análise Fatorial , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Psicolinguística/instrumentação , Psicolinguística/métodos , Psicometria/instrumentação , Distribuição Aleatória , Adulto Jovem
8.
Percept Mot Skills ; 116(2): 456-65, 2013 Apr.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24032322

RESUMO

To date, observations of the scalp-recorded frequency-following response (FFR) to voice pitch have depended on subjective interpretation of the experimenter. The purpose of this study was to develop and evaluate an automated procedure for detecting the presence of a response. Twenty American (9 boys, 1-3 days) and 20 Chinese (10 boys, 1-3 days) neonates were recruited. A Chinese monosyllable that mimicked the English vowel /i/ with a rising pitch (117-166 Hz) was used as the stimulus. Three objective indices (Frequency Error, Tracking Accuracy, and Pitch Strength) were computed from the recorded brain waves and the test results were compared with human judgments to calculate the sensitivity and specificity values. Results demonstrated that the automated procedure produced sensitivity values between 53-90% and specificity values between 80-100%, and could be used to assess the presence of an FFR for neonates who were born in a tonal or non-tonal language environment.


Assuntos
Encéfalo/fisiologia , Testes Neuropsicológicos/normas , Percepção da Altura Sonora/fisiologia , Percepção da Fala/fisiologia , China , Comparação Transcultural , Eletroencefalografia , Feminino , Humanos , Recém-Nascido , Masculino , Psicolinguística/instrumentação , Psicolinguística/métodos , Psicometria/instrumentação , Sensibilidade e Especificidade , Estados Unidos
9.
J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn ; 39(6): 1943-6, 2013 Nov.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23834055

RESUMO

[Correction Notice: An Erratum for this article was reported in Vol 39(6) of Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition (see record 2013-27860-001). In the article there were omissions in Figure 1. All versions of this article have been corrected.] The word frequency paradox refers to the finding that low frequency words are better recognized than high frequency words yet high frequency words are better recalled than low frequency words. Rather than comparing separate groups of low and high frequency words, we sought to quantify the functional relation between word frequency and memory performance across the broad range of frequencies typically used in episodic memory experiments. Here we report that both low frequency and high frequency words are better recalled than midfrequency words. In contrast, we only observe a low frequency advantage when participants were given a subsequent item recognition test. The U-shaped relation between word frequency and recall probability may help to explain inconsistent results in studies using mixed lists with separate groups of high and low frequency words.


Assuntos
Memória Episódica , Rememoração Mental/fisiologia , Reconhecimento Psicológico/fisiologia , Adolescente , Adulto , Humanos , Psicolinguística/instrumentação , Psicolinguística/métodos , Adulto Jovem
10.
J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn ; 39(6): 1990-2001, 2013 Nov.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23834058

RESUMO

The role of morphemic meaning in Chinese word recognition was examined with the masked and unmasked priming paradigms. Target words contained ambiguous morphemes biased toward the dominant or the subordinate meanings. Prime words either contained the same ambiguous morphemes in the subordinate interpretations or were unrelated to the targets. In addition, the relative frequency of the alternative meanings of ambiguous morphemes could be balanced (i.e., the alternative meanings are of similar frequency) or biased (i.e., one of the meanings is used much more frequently). The recognition of subordinate targets was facilitated by the subordinate primes for both balanced and biased items, regardless of the priming procedure. However, the subordinate primes did not facilitate the recognition of dominant targets, except for biased items in masked priming. These results are interpreted as supporting the hypothesis that morphemic meaning is activated to constrain morphological priming even at the early stage of processing. Yet, morpho-semantic activation is modulated by the frequency of the intended morphemic interpretations. Therefore, because of the high frequency of use, the dominant meanings of biased ambiguous morphemes can nevertheless be activated by the subordinate primes.


Assuntos
Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos/fisiologia , Psicolinguística/métodos , Reconhecimento Psicológico/fisiologia , Semântica , Humanos , Mascaramento Perceptivo/fisiologia , Psicolinguística/instrumentação , Priming de Repetição/fisiologia
11.
J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn ; 39(6): 1947-52, 2013 Nov.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23855550

RESUMO

The role of experience in memory, specifically the word frequency (WF) mirror effect showing higher hit rates and lower false alarm rates for low-frequency words, is one of the hallmarks of memory. However, this "regularity of memory" is limited because normative WF has been treated as discrete (low vs. high). We evaluate the extent to which the prototypical WF mirror effect holds when WF is treated as a continuous variable. We find a clear nonmonotonic U-shaped relationship. Hit rates are higher for both low-frequency and high-frequency words. Linear and quadratic regression models were fit to the data at both the item and the participant level, and the quadratic model provided a better fit at both levels. This finding is inconsistent with the empirical and theoretical finding of a mirror effect and requires a novel approach to accounting for the role of experience in episodic memory.


Assuntos
Memória Episódica , Modelos Estatísticos , Reconhecimento Psicológico/fisiologia , Adulto , Associação , Humanos , Psicolinguística/instrumentação , Psicolinguística/métodos , Distribuição Aleatória , Adulto Jovem
12.
J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn ; 39(6): 1793-806, 2013 Nov.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23773181

RESUMO

To investigate bilingual language control, prior language switching studies presented visual objects, which had to be named in different languages, typically indicated by a visual cue. The present study examined language switching of predictable responses by introducing a novel sequence-based language switching paradigm. In 4 experiments, sequential responses (i.e., weekdays, numbers or new sequences) and an alternating language sequence (e.g., L1-L1-L2-L2) were implemented, both of which were memory based. Our data revealed switch costs, showing that a language switch is associated with worse performance compared with a language repetition, and mixing costs, which constitutes the performance difference between pure and mixed language blocks, even while producing entirely predictable responses (i.e., language and concept). Additionally, we found these switch costs with overlearned and new sequences and found that switch costs were reduced with longer preparation time. The obtained data are consistent with a proactive interference account, such as the inhibitory control model.


Assuntos
Função Executiva/fisiologia , Inibição Psicológica , Multilinguismo , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Psicolinguística/instrumentação , Psicolinguística/métodos , Análise e Desempenho de Tarefas , Fatores de Tempo , Adulto Jovem
13.
J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn ; 39(6): 1860-77, 2013 Nov.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23773184

RESUMO

The semantic picture-word interference task has been used to diagnose how speakers resolve competition while selecting words for production. The attentional demands of this resolution process were assessed in 2 dual-task experiments (tone classification followed by picture naming). In Experiment 1, when pictures and distractor words were presented simultaneously, semantic interference was not observed when tasks maximally overlapped. This replicates a key finding from the literature that suggested that semantic picture-word interference does not require capacity-limited central attentional resources and occurs prior to lexical selection, an interpretation that runs counter to the claims of all major theories of word production. In another Experiment 1 condition, when distractors were presented 250 ms after pictures, interference emerged when tasks maximally overlapped. Together, these findings support an account in which interference resolution and lexical selection both require central resources, but the activation of lexical representations from written words does not. Subsequent analysis revealed that discrepant results obtained in previous replication attempts may be attributable to differences in phonological (ir)regularity between languages. In Experiment 2, degree of semantic interference was manipulated using the cumulative semantic interference paradigm. Interference was observed regardless of task overlap, confirming that lexical selection requires central resources. Together, these findings indicate that a lexical selection locus of semantic picture-word interference-and models of word production that assume such a locus-may be retained.


Assuntos
Atenção/fisiologia , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos/fisiologia , Semântica , Adulto , Humanos , Psicolinguística/instrumentação , Psicolinguística/métodos , Distribuição Aleatória , Tempo de Reação/fisiologia , Análise e Desempenho de Tarefas , Fatores de Tempo
14.
J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn ; 39(6): 1842-59, 2013 Nov.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23751009

RESUMO

Word age-of-acquisition (AoA) affects reading. The mapping hypothesis predicts AoA effects when input-output mappings are arbitrary. In Spanish, the orthography-to-phonology mappings required for word naming are consistent; therefore, no AoA effects are expected. Nevertheless, AoA effects have been found, motivating the present investigation of how AoA can affect reading in Spanish. Four experiments were run to examine reading with a factorial design manipulating AoA and frequency. In Experiments 1 and 2 (immediate and speeded naming), only word frequency affected word naming. In Experiment 3 (lexical decision), both AoA and frequency affected word recognition. In Experiment 4 (immediate naming with highly imageable items), both frequency and AoA affected naming. The results suggest that highly imageable items induce a larger reliance on semantics in reading aloud. Such reliance causes faster naming of earlier acquired words because the corresponding concepts have richer visual and sensory features acquired mainly through direct sensory experience.


Assuntos
Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos/fisiologia , Tempo de Reação/fisiologia , Leitura , Adolescente , Adulto , Fatores Etários , Humanos , Desenvolvimento da Linguagem , Psicolinguística/instrumentação , Psicolinguística/métodos , Semântica , Análise e Desempenho de Tarefas , Adulto Jovem
15.
J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn ; 39(6): 1781-92, 2013 Nov.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23647380

RESUMO

The current study investigated the scope of bilingual language control differentiating between whole-language control involving control of an entire lexicon specific to 1 language and lexical-level control involving only a restricted set of recently activated lexical representations. To this end, we tested 60 Dutch-English (Experiment 1) and 64 Chinese-English bilinguals (Experiment 2) on a verbal fluency task in which speakers produced members of letter (or phoneme for Chinese) categories first in 1 language and then members of either (a) the same categories or (b) different categories in their other language. Chinese-English bilinguals also named pictures in both languages. Both bilingual groups showed reduced dominant language fluency after producing exemplars from the same categories in the nondominant language, whereas nondominant language production was not influenced by prior production of words from the same categories in the other language. Chinese-English, but not Dutch-English, bilinguals exhibited similar testing order effects for different letter/phoneme categories. In addition, Chinese-English bilinguals who exhibited significant testing order effects in the repeated categories condition of the fluency task exhibited no such effects when naming repeated pictures after a language switch. These results imply multiple levels of inhibitory control in bilingual language production. Testing order effects in the verbal fluency task pinpoint a lexical locus of bilingual control, and the finding of interference effects for some bilinguals even when different categories are tested across languages further implies a whole-language control process, although the ability to exert such global inhibition may only develop for some types of bilinguals.


Assuntos
Função Executiva/fisiologia , Inibição Psicológica , Multilinguismo , Adulto , Humanos , Psicolinguística/instrumentação , Psicolinguística/métodos , Adulto Jovem
16.
Mem Cognit ; 41(8): 1172-84, 2013 Nov.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23702917

RESUMO

In this study, we examined two issues regarding the role of context in ambiguity resolution: whether access to the contextually appropriate meaning is exhaustive or selective, and whether the contextually inappropriate meaning is inhibited. Participants read texts in which a biased ambiguous word was encountered twice while their eye movements were measured. The context preceding the first encounter varied in the extent to which the subordinate meaning was supported; the context preceding the second encounter always supported the dominant meaning. The findings suggest that lexical access is exhaustive but can be influenced by context, and that the subsequent accessibility of the contextually inappropriate meaning is unaffected by previous selection processes. The results were interpreted in terms of the assumptions of the reordered-access model and activation mechanisms that operate during reading.


Assuntos
Movimentos Oculares/fisiologia , Psicolinguística/métodos , Leitura , Adulto , Medições dos Movimentos Oculares/instrumentação , Humanos , Psicolinguística/instrumentação , Distribuição Aleatória , Adulto Jovem
17.
J Cogn Neurosci ; 25(6): 936-51, 2013 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23631551

RESUMO

In contrast to native language acquisition, adult second-language (L2) acquisition occurs under highly variable learning conditions. Although most adults acquire their L2 at least partially through explicit instruction, as in a classroom setting, many others acquire their L2 primarily through implicit exposure, as is typical of an immersion environment. Whether these differences in acquisition environment play a role in determining the neural mechanisms that are ultimately recruited to process L2 grammar has not been well characterized. This study investigated this issue by comparing the ERP response to novel L2 syntactic rules acquired under conditions of implicit exposure and explicit instruction, using a novel laboratory language-learning paradigm. Native speakers tested on these stimuli showed a biphasic response to syntactic violations, consisting of an earlier negativity followed by a later P600 effect. After merely an hour of training, both implicitly and explicitly trained learners who were capable of detecting grammatical violations also elicited P600 effects. In contrast, learners who were unable to discriminate between grammatically correct and incorrect sentences did not show significant P600 effects. The magnitude of the P600 effect was found to correlate with learners' behavioral proficiency. Behavioral measures revealed that successful learners from both the implicit and explicit groups gained explicit, verbalizable knowledge about the L2 grammar rules. Taken together, these results indicate that late, controlled mechanisms indexed by the P600 play a crucial role in processing a late-learned L2 grammar, regardless of training condition. These findings underscore the remarkable plasticity of later, attention-dependent processes and their importance in lifelong learning.


Assuntos
Eletroencefalografia/métodos , Potenciais Evocados/fisiologia , Idioma , Aprendizagem/fisiologia , Adulto , Eletroencefalografia/instrumentação , Feminino , Humanos , Testes de Linguagem , Aprendizagem/classificação , Masculino , Multilinguismo , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Valor Preditivo dos Testes , Psicolinguística/instrumentação , Psicolinguística/métodos , Adulto Jovem
18.
J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn ; 39(6): 1823-41, 2013 Nov.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23687920

RESUMO

Eye tracking was used to investigate how younger and older (60 or more years) adults use syntactic and semantic information to disambiguate noun/verb (NV) homographs (e.g., park). In event-related potential (ERP) work using the same materials, Lee and Federmeier (2009, 2011) found that young adults elicited a sustained frontal negativity to NV homographs when only syntactic cues were available (i.e., in syntactic prose); this effect was eliminated by semantic constraints. The negativity was only present in older adults with high verbal fluency. The current study shows parallel findings: Young adults exhibit inflated first fixation durations to NV homographs in syntactic prose, but not semantically congruent sentences. This effect is absent in older adults as a group. Verbal fluency modulates the effect in both age groups: High fluency is associated with larger first fixation effects in syntactic prose. Older, but not younger, adults also show significantly increased rereading of the NV homographs in syntactic prose. Verbal fluency modulates this effect as well: High fluency is associated with a reduced tendency to reread, regardless of age. This relationship suggests a trade-off between initial and downstream processing costs for ambiguity during natural reading. Together the eye-tracking and ERP data suggest that effortful meaning selection recruits mechanisms important for suppressing contextually inappropriate meanings, which also slow eye movements. Efficacy of frontotemporal circuitry, as captured by verbal fluency, predicts the success of engaging these mechanisms in both young and older adults. Failure to recruit these processes requires compensatory rereading or leads to comprehension failures (Lee & Federmeier, 2012).


Assuntos
Envelhecimento/fisiologia , Movimentos Oculares/fisiologia , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos/fisiologia , Leitura , Reconhecimento Psicológico/fisiologia , Adolescente , Adulto , Fatores Etários , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Medições dos Movimentos Oculares/instrumentação , Feminino , Fixação Ocular/fisiologia , Humanos , Individualidade , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Psicolinguística/instrumentação , Psicolinguística/métodos , Distribuição Aleatória , Semântica , Adulto Jovem
19.
Psychon Bull Rev ; 20(5): 981-7, 2013 Oct.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23456328

RESUMO

Models of spoken word recognition assume that words are represented as sequences of phonemes. We evaluated this assumption by examining phonemic anadromes, words that share the same phonemes but differ in their order (e.g., sub and bus). Using the visual-world paradigm, we found that listeners show more fixations to anadromes (e.g., sub when bus is the target) than to unrelated words (well) and to words that share the same vowel but not the same set of phonemes (sun). This contrasts with the predictions of existing models and suggests that words are not defined as strict sequences of phonemes.


Assuntos
Movimentos Oculares/fisiologia , Reconhecimento Psicológico/fisiologia , Percepção da Fala/fisiologia , Adulto , Medições dos Movimentos Oculares/instrumentação , Humanos , Psicolinguística/instrumentação , Psicolinguística/métodos , Fatores de Tempo , Adulto Jovem
20.
Am J Intellect Dev Disabil ; 118(2): 108-23, 2013 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23464609

RESUMO

Previous research has suggested that individuals with intellectual disability (ID) underperform in several areas of reading compared to mental age-matched peers. However, it is unclear how they compare on orthographic aspects of reading, which have to do with learning and matching the specific letter patterns in words. The leading approach to understanding orthographic learning is the self-teaching hypothesis, which suggests that orthographic learning is acquired through the experience of phonologically recoding words. The present study was a first test of the self-teaching hypothesis for individuals with ID in comparison to a group of typically developing children matched on verbal mental age. Results indicated that both groups were able to self-teach.


Assuntos
Deficiência Intelectual/fisiopatologia , Aprendizagem/fisiologia , Leitura , Adolescente , Criança , Feminino , Humanos , Deficiência Intelectual/psicologia , Masculino , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Fonética , Psicolinguística/instrumentação , Psicolinguística/métodos
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA
...